Paper Search Console

Home Search Page About Contact

Journal Title

Title of Journal: Exp Brain Res

Search In Journal Title:

Abbravation: Experimental Brain Research

Search In Journal Abbravation:

Publisher

Springer-Verlag

Search In Publisher:

DOI

10.1016/0003-2670(94)00345-9

Search In DOI:

ISSN

1432-1106

Search In ISSN:
Search In Title Of Papers:

The Brentano illusion influences goaldirected mov

Authors: Denise D J de Grave Eli Brenner Jeroen B J Smeets
Publish Date: 2008/11/15
Volume: 193, Issue: 3, Pages: 421-427
PDF Link

Abstract

Recently Gonzalez et al J Neurophys 953496–3501 2006 reported that movements with the left hand are more susceptible to visual size illusions than are those with the right hand We hypothesized that this might be because proprioceptive information about the position of the left hand is less precise If so the difference between the hands should become clearer when vision of the hand is removed so that subjects can only rely on proprioception to locate their hand We tested whether this was so by letting righthanded subjects make openloop pointing movements within an illusory context with and without vision of their hand On average the illusion influenced the left and the right hand to the same extent irrespective of the visibility of the hand There were some systematic differences between the hands within certain regions of space but these were not consistent across subjects We conclude that there is no fundamental difference between the hands in susceptibility to the Brentano illusionWhen investigating the effect of visual illusions on visuomotor performance tasks are normally performed with the dominant hand For most people this is the right one Gonzalez et al 2006 examined how visual illusions affect grasping movements with the left and the right hand in both left and righthanded subjects They showed subjects small objects which they had to pick up from within an illusory context The illusion affected maximum grip aperture more when grasping movements were performed with the left hand than when they were performed with the right hand irrespective of the subject’s handednessAlthough this seems a rather strange result one may not simply expect lefthanded subjects to behave in a mirrorsymmetric manner to righthanded ones When left and righthanded subjects are asked to grasp small objects with a precision grip righthanded subjects prefer to use their dominant hand whereas lefthanded subjects use their right hand about half of the time Gonzalez et al 2007 see also Stins et al 2001 Several other studies have also shown differences in performance between the arms reviewed in Goble and Brown 2008 Important for the present study is the finding of Van Beers et al 1998 who demonstrated that the nonvisual proprioceptive information about the position of the hand is not only more precise for the right hand for righthanded subjects but also for lefthanded onesThe asymmetry in proprioceptive accuracy is the basis of a possible explanation for the larger effect of illusions on the left hand A lower precision in proprioception for the left than the right hand means that egocentric information about the location of the left hand is less precise than egocentric information about the location of the right hand As subjects are likely to rely less on imprecise information van Beers et al 2002 they will rely more on other sources of information such as movement distance when using their left hand and therefore be more strongly influenced by illusions of length de Grave et al 2004Whether this can account for the influence on grip aperture in the grasping study of Gonzalez et al 2006 depends on how one thinks that the effect of context items on grip aperture arises The effect of the illusion implies that we must reject the idea Smeets and Brenner 1999 that egocentric information about the positions of the contact points is the only information that is used A first option is that allocentric information about the object’s size which is influenced by the illusion is also used Jeannerod 1981 Alternatively elements in the surrounding context may be regarded as obstacles that subjects have to take care not to collide with Biegstraaten et al 2007 de Grave et al 2005 Haffenden and Goodale 2000 Both giving size more consideration and taking more care to avoid obstacles when moving with the left hand would be consistent with the effect of the illusory context having more influence on the less precise left hand than on the more precise right handIf the difference in proprioceptive precision underlies the difference in illusion effects between the hands the latter difference should increase when subjects rely more on proprioception rather than vision to localize the hand In this study we investigate whether this is so by comparing the effect of an illusion when visibility of the hand is removed with that when the hand remains visible When the hand is invisible subjects cannot rely on visual information regarding the position of their hand but have to use proprioceptive information which is less precise for the left hand Since grasping is known to be influenced by factors such as posture Cuijpers et al 2004 and the consistency of haptic feedback Cuijpers et al 2008 we chose to use a simpler pointing task on a graphics tablet We chose the Brentano illusion because it is known to affect pointing movements considerably but still much less than it affects size judgments so we can be sure that we will be able to detect small changes in the effect of the illusionThe methods are very similar to two conditions of an earlier experiment in which subjects had to move a pen to a target that disappeared as soon as the pen started to move de Grave et al 2004 In the first condition the hand was continuously visible whereas in the second condition the hand was invisible throughout the experiment The critical difference with the study of de Grave et al 2004 is that subjects performed each condition twice once with their left hand and once with their right handTen subjects took part in this study All subjects performed both conditions They all had normal or correctedtonormal vision and were righthanded according to the Edinburgh Handedness Inventory score Oldfield 1971 One subject had a score of 80 the score for all the others was 100 The study was approved by the ethics committee of the Faculty of Human Movement Science


Keywords:

References


.
Search In Abstract Of Papers:
Other Papers In This Journal:

  1. Effects of electrode penetrations into the abducens nucleus of the monkey: eye movement recordings and histopathological evaluation of the nuclei and lateral rectus muscles
  2. Task dependent gain regulation of spinal circuits projecting to the human flexor carpi radialis
  3. Mirror apraxia affects the peripersonal mirror space. A combined lesion and cerebral activation study
  4. Reduced intracortical inhibition during the foreperiod of a warned reaction time task
  5. Sensitivity to hierarchical relations among affordances in the assembly of asymmetric tools
  6. Modulation of somatosensory evoked potentials during force generation and relaxation
  7. Temporal and spatial constraints of action effect on sensory binding
  8. Is perception of upper body orientation based on the inertia tensor? Normogravity versus microgravity conditions
  9. Is gaze following purely reflexive or goal-directed instead? Revisiting the automaticity of orienting attention by gaze cues
  10. Effect of pitch–space correspondence on sound-induced visual motion perception
  11. Movement-related modulation of vibrotactile detection thresholds in the human orofacial system
  12. Left visual neglect: is the disengage deficit space- or object-based?
  13. Manual obstacle avoidance takes into account visual uncertainty, motor noise, and biomechanical costs
  14. Irregular head movement patterns in whiplash patients during a trajectory task
  15. Glutamatergic systems in the bed nucleus of the stria terminalis, effects on cardiovascular system
  16. Photolytic flash-induced intercellular calcium waves using caged calcium ionophore in cultured astrocytes from newborn rats
  17. Self-motion perception training: thresholds improve in the light but not in the dark
  18. Dynamic visual–vestibular integration during goal directed human locomotion
  19. Central and peripheral psychophysiological responses to trauma-related cues in subclinical posttraumatic stress disorder: a pilot study
  20. Cerebellar cortical activity in the cat anterior lobe during hindlimb stepping
  21. Information processing in long delay memory-guided saccades: further insights from TMS
  22. The disynaptic group I inhibition between wrist flexor and extensor muscles revisited in humans
  23. Information processing in long delay memory-guided saccades: further insights from TMS
  24. Tool use changes multisensory interactions in seconds: evidence from the crossmodal congruency task
  25. Fast muscle responses to an unexpected foot-in-hole scenario, evoked in the context of prior knowledge of the potential perturbation
  26. Co-induction of αB-crystallin and MAPKAPK-2 in astrocytes in the penumbra after transient focal cerebral ischemia
  27. Does Parkinson’s disease affect judgement about another person’s action?
  28. Task relevance regulates the interaction between reward expectation and emotion
  29. Brainstem processing of vestibular sensory exafference: implications for motion sickness etiology
  30. Differences in cortical activation during smooth pursuit and saccadic eye movements following cerebellar lesions
  31. Non-invasive stimulation of the vibrissal pad improves recovery of whisking function after simultaneous lesion of the facial and infraorbital nerves in rats
  32. Properties and axonal trajectories of posterior semicircular canal nerve-activated vestibulospinal neurons
  33. Sensory information in perceptual-motor sequence learning: visual and/or tactile stimuli
  34. An autoradiographic study of efferent connections of the globus pallidus in Macaca mulatta
  35. Discrete and cyclical units of action in a mixed target pair aiming task
  36. Electrophysiological correlates of short-latency afferent inhibition: a combined EEG and TMS study
  37. Task-related variations in the surface EMG of the human first dorsal interosseous muscle
  38. Remapping of place cell firing patterns after maze rotations
  39. Effects of implicit visual feedback distortion on human gait
  40. Muscle synergies during voluntary body sway: combining across-trials and within-a-trial analyses
  41. Attention to touch weakens audiovisual speech integration
  42. Time of transplantation and cell preparation determine neural stem cell survival in a mouse model of Huntington’s disease
  43. Number generation bias after action observation
  44. The functional significance of velocity storage and its dependence on gravity
  45. Evidence of impaired neuromuscular responses in the support leg to a destabilizing swing phase perturbation in hemiparetic gait
  46. Acquiring and adapting a novel audiomotor map in human grasping
  47. Postcentral neurons with covert receptive fields in conscious macaque monkeys: their selective responsiveness to simultaneous two-point stimuli applied to discrete oral portions
  48. Postcentral neurons with covert receptive fields in conscious macaque monkeys: their selective responsiveness to simultaneous two-point stimuli applied to discrete oral portions
  49. High frequency stimulation of the subthalamic nucleus modulates neurotransmission in limbic brain regions of the rat
  50. Group II excitations from plantar foot muscles to human leg and thigh motoneurones
  51. Limits to human movement planning with delayed and unpredictable onset of needed information
  52. Distinct patterns of hippocampal formation activity associated with different spatial tasks: a Fos imaging study in rats
  53. Processing of visual information compromises the ability of older adults to control novel fine motor tasks
  54. Automated postural responses are modified in a functional manner by instruction
  55. Shifts of attention bias awareness of voluntary and reflexive eye movements
  56. Can imagery become reality?
  57. Saccadic adaptation shifts the pre-saccadic attention focus
  58. Keep looking ahead? Re-direction of visual fixation does not always occur during an unpredictable obstacle avoidance task
  59. Hitting moving targets
  60. Excitotoxic injury to thoracolumbar gray matter alters sympathetic activation and thermal pain sensitivity
  61. Cathodal transcranial direct current stimulation on posterior parietal cortex disrupts visuo-spatial processing in the contralateral visual field
  62. Adaptation of motor control strategies to environmental cues in a pursuit-tracking task
  63. Wideband phase locking to modulated whisker vibration point to a temporal code for texture in the rat’s barrel cortex
  64. Observing social gestures: an fMRI study
  65. Vector inversion diminishes the online control of antisaccades
  66. Chronic intracerebroventricular delivery of the secretory phospholipase A 2 inhibitor, 12- epi -scalaradial, does not improve outcome after focal cerebral ischemia–reperfusion in rats
  67. Active eye fixation performance in 940 young men: effects of IQ, schizotypy, anxiety and depression
  68. Precision-grip force changes in the anatomical and prosthetic limb during predictable load increases
  69. Hippocampal contribution to early and later stages of implicit motor sequence learning
  70. Interaction between vibration-evoked proprioceptive illusions and mirror-evoked visual illusions in an arm-matching task
  71. Visual signals contribute to the coding of gaze direction
  72. Extending Fitts’ Law to three-dimensional obstacle-avoidance movements: support for the posture-based motion planning model
  73. (De)synchronization of advanced visual information and ball flight characteristics constrains emergent information–movement couplings during one-handed catching
  74. Two illusions of perceived orientation: one fools all of the people some of the time; the other fools all of the people all of the time
  75. Experimental hypervigilance changes the intensity/unpleasantness ratio of pressure sensations: evidence for the generalized hypervigilance hypothesis
  76. Neural correlates of the essence of conscious conflict: fMRI of sustaining incompatible intentions
  77. Force fluctuations while pressing and moving against high- and low-friction touch screen surfaces
  78. Effects of motor preparation and spatial attention on corticospinal excitability in a delayed-response paradigm
  79. The effect of high-frequency cutaneous vibration on different inputs subserving detection of joint movement
  80. The influence of response competition on cerebral asymmetries for processing hierarchical stimuli revealed by ERP recordings
  81. Priming tool actions: Are real objects more effective primes than pictures?
  82. Time course and specificity of sensory-motor alpha modulation during the observation of hand motor acts and gestures: a high density EEG study
  83. Human discrimination of rotational velocities
  84. Egocentric and allocentric reference frames for catching a falling object
  85. Suppression of motor evoked potentials in biceps brachii preceding pronator contraction
  86. Tactile feedback contributes to consistency of finger movements during typing
  87. Effects of overshadowing on conditioned and unconditioned nausea in a rotation paradigm with humans
  88. How a new behavioral pattern is stabilized with learning determines its persistence and flexibility in memory
  89. Differential effects of absent visual feedback control on gait variability during different locomotion speeds
  90. Cervical muscle response to trunk flexion in whiplash-type lateral impacts
  91. Durability of classification and action learning: differences revealed using ex-Gaussian distribution analysis
  92. Learning a stick-balancing task involves task-specific coupling between posture and hand displacements
  93. Learning a stick-balancing task involves task-specific coupling between posture and hand displacements
  94. Gesture imitation in musicians and non-musicians
  95. The strategies to regulate and to modulate the propulsive forces during gait initiation in lower limb amputees
  96. Prehension synergies: trial-to-trial variability and hierarchical organization of stable performance
  97. Corticomotor control of lumbar multifidus muscles is impaired in chronic low back pain: concurrent evidence from ultrasound imaging and double-pulse transcranial magnetic stimulation
  98. The origins of neuromuscular fatigue post-stroke
  99. Amplitude and direction errors in kinesthetic pointing
  100. The preferred sensory direction of muscle spindle primary endings influences the velocity coding of two-dimensional limb movements in humans
  101. The role of long-term and short-term familiarity in visual and haptic face recognition
  102. The role of long-term and short-term familiarity in visual and haptic face recognition
  103. The role of connexins in the differentiation of NT2 cells in Sertoli-NT2 cell tissue constructs grown in the rotating wall bioreactor
  104. Primary sensorimotor cortex activation with task-performance after fatiguing hand exercise
  105. Effects of visual deprivation on intra-limb coordination during walking in children and adults
  106. Motor unit number in a small facial muscle, dilator naris
  107. Dissociable contributions of motor-execution and action-observation to intramanual transfer
  108. Conflict with vision diminishes proprioceptive adaptation to muscle vibration
  109. Effect of a visual distractor on line bisection
  110. Corticomotor excitability during a choice-hand reaction time task
  111. Force coordination during bimanual task performance in Parkinson’s disease
  112. Effects of biomechanical and task constraints on the organization of movement in precision aiming
  113. Visual responses of thalamic neurons depending on the direction of gaze and the position of targets in space
  114. Modulation of motor unit discharge rate and H-reflex amplitude during submaximal fatigue of the human soleus muscle
  115. Subcortical reorganization in amyotrophic lateral sclerosis
  116. Fitts’s Law violation and motor imagery: are imagined movements truthful or lawful?
  117. Apomorphine-susceptible rats and apomorphine-unsusceptible rats differ in the tyrosine hydroxylase-immunoreactive network in the nucleus accumbens core and shell
  118. Factors influencing variability in load forces in a tripod grasp
  119. Perceived finger orientation is biased towards functional task spaces
  120. Eye–hand coupling is not the cause of manual return movements when searching
  121. Vibratory noise to the fingertip enhances balance improvement associated with light touch
  122. Haptic discrimination of two-dimensional angles: influence of exploratory strategy
  123. Afferent-mediated modulation of the soleus muscle activity during the stance phase of human walking
  124. Interaction between the noradrenergic and serotonergic systems in locomotor hyperactivity and striatal expression of Fos induced by amphetamine in rats
  125. Recall of observed actions modulates the end-state comfort effect just like recall of one’s own actions
  126. Non-visually evoked activity of isthmo-optic neurons in awake, head-unrestrained quail
  127. Auditory-motor mapping for pitch control in singers and nonsingers
  128. Cerebral cortical processing of swallowing in older adults
  129. Instruction-dependent modulation of the long-latency stretch reflex is associated with indicators of startle
  130. Active linear head motion improves dynamic visual acuity in pursuing a high-speed moving object
  131. Right but not left angular gyrus modulates the metric component of the mental body representation: a tDCS study

Search Result: