Paper Search Console

Home Search Page About Contact

Journal Title

Title of Journal: Exp Brain Res

Search In Journal Title:

Abbravation: Experimental Brain Research

Search In Journal Abbravation:

Publisher

Springer-Verlag

Search In Publisher:

DOI

10.1002/nadc.19580061006

Search In DOI:

ISSN

1432-1106

Search In ISSN:
Search In Title Of Papers:

Sensory information in perceptualmotor sequence l

Authors: Elger L Abrahamse Rob H J van der Lubbe Willem B Verwey
Publish Date: 2009/06/30
Volume: 197, Issue: 2, Pages: 175-183
PDF Link

Abstract

Sequence learning in serial reaction time SRT tasks has been investigated mostly with unimodal stimulus presentation This approach disregards the possibility that sequence acquisition may be guided by multiple sources of sensory information simultaneously In the current study we trained participants in a SRT task with visual only tactile only or bimodal visual and tactile stimulus presentation Sequence performance for the bimodal and visual only training groups was similar while both performed better than the tactile only training group In a subsequent transfer phase participants from all three training groups were tested in conditions with visual tactile and bimodal stimulus presentation Sequence performance between the visual only and bimodal training groups again was highly similar across these identical stimulus conditions indicating that the addition of tactile stimuli did not benefit the bimodal training group Additionally comparing across identical stimulus conditions in the transfer phase showed that the lesser sequence performance from the tactile only group during training probably did not reflect a difference in sequence learning but rather just a difference in expression of the sequence knowledgeOne crucial aspect of motor performance is the ability to learn sequences of movements Typically motor sequence learning is studied using buttonpressing tasks such as the serial reaction time SRT task or the discrete sequence production DSP task in which participants are required to respond to single stimuli presented visually on a screen However in daily life we simultaneously encounter multiple sources of sensory information across different modalities1 Whereas the effect of bimodal congruent stimuli has been extensively explored with respect to trial by trial performance in simple and choice reaction time RT tasks eg Frens et al 1995 Giard and Peronnet 1999 Rowland and Stein 2007 far less is known about the impact of such stimulus pairs on sequence learning across trials In the current study we explored whether congruent and temporally synchronized visual and tactile stimuli enhance learning of a sequence of actions in an SRT taskIn its basic form the SRT task requires participants to respond fast and accurately by pressing the buttons corresponding to the locations of successively presented visual stimuli eg Nissen and Bullemer 1987 Unbeknownst to them however stimulus presentation is structured and reaction time RT decreases with practice To differentiate sequence learning from general practice effects a random block of stimuli is inserted at the end of the practice phase The cost in terms of RT and/or accuracy ie sequence effect of this random block relative to its surrounding sequence blocks serves as an index for sequence learning Often participants are unable to express their sequence knowledge in other ways than reflected by RT and accuracy scores and learning is said to partly have taken place implicitlyThe nature of the representation underlying implicit learning is still being debated Whereas responsebased learning is the dominant and best documented account in literature eg BischoffGrethe et al 2004 Grafton et al 1995 Nattkemper and Prinz 1997 Rüsseler and Rösler 2000 Willingham 1999 Willingham et al 2000 recently support is mounting also for sequence learning that involves stimulus features responseeffect learning eg Stöcker et al 2003 Ziessler and Nattkemper 2001 and perceptual location learning eg Deroost and Soetens 2006 Mayr 1996 Remillard 2003 This prompts investigation on the effects that different sensory environments have upon sequence learning eg Abrahamse et al 2008 Jiménez and Vázquez 2008 Robertson and PascualLeone 2001 Robertson et al 2001 Robertson and colleagues Robertson and PascualLeone 2001 Robertson et al 2001 recognized the fact that we are continuously surrounded by multiple sources of sensory information in the real world They explored sequence learning in an SRT task in which required responses were signaled through redundant position and color cues They reported that compared to either single cue condition position or color sequence learning was augmented with combined position and color cuesThe latter supports the notion that perceptualmotor skill acquisition can benefit from multiple sources of congruent information at least within the visual domain However it remains unclear whether these findings would extend to congruent stimuli presented through different sensory modalities It is known from simple detection and choice RT tasks that presenting congruent stimuli across modalities sometimes results in additive or even superadditive sensory interactions eg Miller and Ulrich 2003 Santangelo et al 2008 Stein and Meredith 1993 indicating that information from the different sensory sources gets integrated along the timecourse of SR processing This integration of bimodal stimuli has been found to occur both at early and later sensoryperceptual processing stages and seems to be conditional on the spatial proximity and/or temporal synchrony of the separate stimuli eg Atteveldt et al 2007 Harrington and Peck 1998 Helbig and Ernst 2007 TederSälejärvi et al 2005 Murray et al 2005 From the notion that sensory information plays a role in the formation of the representations underlying sequence learning eg Clegg 2005 Remillard 2003 one may expect that the enriched perceptual events that follow from integrated bimodal stimuli produce stronger sequence representations than those obtained with single stimuliRecently Abrahamse et al 2008 introduced a new version of the SRT task in which stimuli were presented tactilely to the fingers and learning was compared to the typical visual version of the SRT task Sequence learning was reliably observed for both stimulus conditions but it appeared to be better for the condition with visual stimuli In a subsequent transfer phase for both visual and tactile training groups we assessed transfer of sequence learning to the other modality It seemed that transfer was perfect from tactile to visual stimuli but only partial the other way around As we will elaborate on below though these findings deserve some closer inspection because of methodological issuesIn the current study we extended the study of Abrahamse et al 2008 by adding a condition in which congruent visual and tactile stimuli were presented simultaneously Hence participants were trained either with congruent visual and tactile stimuli bimodal training group with visual stimuli only visual only training group or tactile stimuli only tactile only training group This allowed us to investigate the employment by the cognitive system of redundant visual and tactile stimuli each of which has been shown to produce sequence learning when presented alone ie Abrahamse et al 2008 In a subsequent transfer phase transfer to all three stimulus conditions ie visual tactile and bimodal transfer test was assessed for each training group The transfer of sequence knowledge to new conditions is one of the major tools in exploring the nature of sequence learning Clegg et al 1998 Thus exploring whether sequence knowledge acquired in one stimulus condition could readily be applied to different stimulus conditions provides indications on the nature of the representation underlying sequence learning In this respect the transfer test to the initial training condition offered a clear baseline for transfer Additionally comparing across identical stimulus conditions at transfer allows controlling for effects of the training stimulus condition on just the expression of sequence knowledge It has been shown a number of times that sequence knowledge is better expressed under some experimental conditions than others eg Deroost et al 2009 Frensch et al 19982 Finally and closely related to the latter assessing performance across one or more identical stimulus conditions allows comparing performances with more or less similar baseline RTs thereby circumventing the debate of whether differences in baseline RTs should be considered in determining the amount of sequence learning some authors have chosen to normalize the data for baseline differences eg Robertson and PascualLeone 2001We would like to stress that for both the training and transfer phase our main interest was whether the bimodal training group would benefit from the addition of tactile stimuli in comparison to the visual only training group The bimodal training group was logically expected to show better sequence learning than the tactile only training group due to the availability of visual stimuli since visual stimuli have been shown to produce better sequence learning than tactile stimuli only Abrahamse et al 2008


Keywords:

References


.
Search In Abstract Of Papers:
Other Papers In This Journal:

  1. Effects of electrode penetrations into the abducens nucleus of the monkey: eye movement recordings and histopathological evaluation of the nuclei and lateral rectus muscles
  2. Task dependent gain regulation of spinal circuits projecting to the human flexor carpi radialis
  3. Mirror apraxia affects the peripersonal mirror space. A combined lesion and cerebral activation study
  4. Reduced intracortical inhibition during the foreperiod of a warned reaction time task
  5. Sensitivity to hierarchical relations among affordances in the assembly of asymmetric tools
  6. Modulation of somatosensory evoked potentials during force generation and relaxation
  7. Temporal and spatial constraints of action effect on sensory binding
  8. Is perception of upper body orientation based on the inertia tensor? Normogravity versus microgravity conditions
  9. Is gaze following purely reflexive or goal-directed instead? Revisiting the automaticity of orienting attention by gaze cues
  10. Effect of pitch–space correspondence on sound-induced visual motion perception
  11. Movement-related modulation of vibrotactile detection thresholds in the human orofacial system
  12. Left visual neglect: is the disengage deficit space- or object-based?
  13. Manual obstacle avoidance takes into account visual uncertainty, motor noise, and biomechanical costs
  14. Irregular head movement patterns in whiplash patients during a trajectory task
  15. Glutamatergic systems in the bed nucleus of the stria terminalis, effects on cardiovascular system
  16. Photolytic flash-induced intercellular calcium waves using caged calcium ionophore in cultured astrocytes from newborn rats
  17. Self-motion perception training: thresholds improve in the light but not in the dark
  18. Dynamic visual–vestibular integration during goal directed human locomotion
  19. Central and peripheral psychophysiological responses to trauma-related cues in subclinical posttraumatic stress disorder: a pilot study
  20. Cerebellar cortical activity in the cat anterior lobe during hindlimb stepping
  21. Information processing in long delay memory-guided saccades: further insights from TMS
  22. The disynaptic group I inhibition between wrist flexor and extensor muscles revisited in humans
  23. Information processing in long delay memory-guided saccades: further insights from TMS
  24. Tool use changes multisensory interactions in seconds: evidence from the crossmodal congruency task
  25. Fast muscle responses to an unexpected foot-in-hole scenario, evoked in the context of prior knowledge of the potential perturbation
  26. Co-induction of αB-crystallin and MAPKAPK-2 in astrocytes in the penumbra after transient focal cerebral ischemia
  27. Does Parkinson’s disease affect judgement about another person’s action?
  28. Task relevance regulates the interaction between reward expectation and emotion
  29. Brainstem processing of vestibular sensory exafference: implications for motion sickness etiology
  30. Differences in cortical activation during smooth pursuit and saccadic eye movements following cerebellar lesions
  31. Non-invasive stimulation of the vibrissal pad improves recovery of whisking function after simultaneous lesion of the facial and infraorbital nerves in rats
  32. Properties and axonal trajectories of posterior semicircular canal nerve-activated vestibulospinal neurons
  33. An autoradiographic study of efferent connections of the globus pallidus in Macaca mulatta
  34. Discrete and cyclical units of action in a mixed target pair aiming task
  35. Electrophysiological correlates of short-latency afferent inhibition: a combined EEG and TMS study
  36. Task-related variations in the surface EMG of the human first dorsal interosseous muscle
  37. Remapping of place cell firing patterns after maze rotations
  38. Effects of implicit visual feedback distortion on human gait
  39. Muscle synergies during voluntary body sway: combining across-trials and within-a-trial analyses
  40. Attention to touch weakens audiovisual speech integration
  41. Time of transplantation and cell preparation determine neural stem cell survival in a mouse model of Huntington’s disease
  42. Number generation bias after action observation
  43. The functional significance of velocity storage and its dependence on gravity
  44. Evidence of impaired neuromuscular responses in the support leg to a destabilizing swing phase perturbation in hemiparetic gait
  45. Acquiring and adapting a novel audiomotor map in human grasping
  46. Postcentral neurons with covert receptive fields in conscious macaque monkeys: their selective responsiveness to simultaneous two-point stimuli applied to discrete oral portions
  47. Postcentral neurons with covert receptive fields in conscious macaque monkeys: their selective responsiveness to simultaneous two-point stimuli applied to discrete oral portions
  48. High frequency stimulation of the subthalamic nucleus modulates neurotransmission in limbic brain regions of the rat
  49. Group II excitations from plantar foot muscles to human leg and thigh motoneurones
  50. Limits to human movement planning with delayed and unpredictable onset of needed information
  51. Distinct patterns of hippocampal formation activity associated with different spatial tasks: a Fos imaging study in rats
  52. Processing of visual information compromises the ability of older adults to control novel fine motor tasks
  53. Automated postural responses are modified in a functional manner by instruction
  54. Shifts of attention bias awareness of voluntary and reflexive eye movements
  55. Can imagery become reality?
  56. Saccadic adaptation shifts the pre-saccadic attention focus
  57. Keep looking ahead? Re-direction of visual fixation does not always occur during an unpredictable obstacle avoidance task
  58. Hitting moving targets
  59. Excitotoxic injury to thoracolumbar gray matter alters sympathetic activation and thermal pain sensitivity
  60. Cathodal transcranial direct current stimulation on posterior parietal cortex disrupts visuo-spatial processing in the contralateral visual field
  61. Adaptation of motor control strategies to environmental cues in a pursuit-tracking task
  62. Wideband phase locking to modulated whisker vibration point to a temporal code for texture in the rat’s barrel cortex
  63. Observing social gestures: an fMRI study
  64. Vector inversion diminishes the online control of antisaccades
  65. Chronic intracerebroventricular delivery of the secretory phospholipase A 2 inhibitor, 12- epi -scalaradial, does not improve outcome after focal cerebral ischemia–reperfusion in rats
  66. Active eye fixation performance in 940 young men: effects of IQ, schizotypy, anxiety and depression
  67. Precision-grip force changes in the anatomical and prosthetic limb during predictable load increases
  68. Hippocampal contribution to early and later stages of implicit motor sequence learning
  69. Interaction between vibration-evoked proprioceptive illusions and mirror-evoked visual illusions in an arm-matching task
  70. Visual signals contribute to the coding of gaze direction
  71. Extending Fitts’ Law to three-dimensional obstacle-avoidance movements: support for the posture-based motion planning model
  72. (De)synchronization of advanced visual information and ball flight characteristics constrains emergent information–movement couplings during one-handed catching
  73. Two illusions of perceived orientation: one fools all of the people some of the time; the other fools all of the people all of the time
  74. Experimental hypervigilance changes the intensity/unpleasantness ratio of pressure sensations: evidence for the generalized hypervigilance hypothesis
  75. Neural correlates of the essence of conscious conflict: fMRI of sustaining incompatible intentions
  76. Force fluctuations while pressing and moving against high- and low-friction touch screen surfaces
  77. Effects of motor preparation and spatial attention on corticospinal excitability in a delayed-response paradigm
  78. The effect of high-frequency cutaneous vibration on different inputs subserving detection of joint movement
  79. The influence of response competition on cerebral asymmetries for processing hierarchical stimuli revealed by ERP recordings
  80. Priming tool actions: Are real objects more effective primes than pictures?
  81. Time course and specificity of sensory-motor alpha modulation during the observation of hand motor acts and gestures: a high density EEG study
  82. Human discrimination of rotational velocities
  83. Egocentric and allocentric reference frames for catching a falling object
  84. Suppression of motor evoked potentials in biceps brachii preceding pronator contraction
  85. Tactile feedback contributes to consistency of finger movements during typing
  86. Effects of overshadowing on conditioned and unconditioned nausea in a rotation paradigm with humans
  87. How a new behavioral pattern is stabilized with learning determines its persistence and flexibility in memory
  88. Differential effects of absent visual feedback control on gait variability during different locomotion speeds
  89. Cervical muscle response to trunk flexion in whiplash-type lateral impacts
  90. Durability of classification and action learning: differences revealed using ex-Gaussian distribution analysis
  91. Learning a stick-balancing task involves task-specific coupling between posture and hand displacements
  92. Learning a stick-balancing task involves task-specific coupling between posture and hand displacements
  93. Gesture imitation in musicians and non-musicians
  94. The strategies to regulate and to modulate the propulsive forces during gait initiation in lower limb amputees
  95. Prehension synergies: trial-to-trial variability and hierarchical organization of stable performance
  96. Corticomotor control of lumbar multifidus muscles is impaired in chronic low back pain: concurrent evidence from ultrasound imaging and double-pulse transcranial magnetic stimulation
  97. The origins of neuromuscular fatigue post-stroke
  98. Amplitude and direction errors in kinesthetic pointing
  99. The preferred sensory direction of muscle spindle primary endings influences the velocity coding of two-dimensional limb movements in humans
  100. The role of long-term and short-term familiarity in visual and haptic face recognition
  101. The role of long-term and short-term familiarity in visual and haptic face recognition
  102. The role of connexins in the differentiation of NT2 cells in Sertoli-NT2 cell tissue constructs grown in the rotating wall bioreactor
  103. Primary sensorimotor cortex activation with task-performance after fatiguing hand exercise
  104. Effects of visual deprivation on intra-limb coordination during walking in children and adults
  105. Motor unit number in a small facial muscle, dilator naris
  106. Dissociable contributions of motor-execution and action-observation to intramanual transfer
  107. Conflict with vision diminishes proprioceptive adaptation to muscle vibration
  108. Effect of a visual distractor on line bisection
  109. Corticomotor excitability during a choice-hand reaction time task
  110. Force coordination during bimanual task performance in Parkinson’s disease
  111. Effects of biomechanical and task constraints on the organization of movement in precision aiming
  112. Visual responses of thalamic neurons depending on the direction of gaze and the position of targets in space
  113. Modulation of motor unit discharge rate and H-reflex amplitude during submaximal fatigue of the human soleus muscle
  114. Subcortical reorganization in amyotrophic lateral sclerosis
  115. Fitts’s Law violation and motor imagery: are imagined movements truthful or lawful?
  116. Apomorphine-susceptible rats and apomorphine-unsusceptible rats differ in the tyrosine hydroxylase-immunoreactive network in the nucleus accumbens core and shell
  117. Factors influencing variability in load forces in a tripod grasp
  118. Perceived finger orientation is biased towards functional task spaces
  119. Eye–hand coupling is not the cause of manual return movements when searching
  120. Vibratory noise to the fingertip enhances balance improvement associated with light touch
  121. Haptic discrimination of two-dimensional angles: influence of exploratory strategy
  122. Afferent-mediated modulation of the soleus muscle activity during the stance phase of human walking
  123. Interaction between the noradrenergic and serotonergic systems in locomotor hyperactivity and striatal expression of Fos induced by amphetamine in rats
  124. Recall of observed actions modulates the end-state comfort effect just like recall of one’s own actions
  125. Non-visually evoked activity of isthmo-optic neurons in awake, head-unrestrained quail
  126. Auditory-motor mapping for pitch control in singers and nonsingers
  127. Cerebral cortical processing of swallowing in older adults
  128. Instruction-dependent modulation of the long-latency stretch reflex is associated with indicators of startle
  129. Active linear head motion improves dynamic visual acuity in pursuing a high-speed moving object
  130. The Brentano illusion influences goal-directed movements of the left and right hand to the same extent
  131. Right but not left angular gyrus modulates the metric component of the mental body representation: a tDCS study

Search Result: